which kind of old are we?

How long is a year in evolutionary time? Well, to find out we’d have to count every participant, at every scale. We would then know the number of lifeYears in a single solar year on Earth. Seeing this absurd and magical number, which I suppose to be greater than 10 to the 100th power, would probably leave us startled. Each year it would be a very different number, however absurdly large it was. Many ‘individuals’ would contain many scales of constituents — thus, many scales of lifeTime. Noticing this means that a species that is 100 years old, is actually 100 times the (total number of participants who existed for each year) years old.

In such a game conservation of accruals happens in the connectivities and bodies of individuals and populations of every kind and scale. Members of a species like ours, one which specializes in symbolic conservation of accrued complexity, are biological, cognitive, and psychological examples of precisely this form of temporal relation: one which uses time like a scalar accelerator. As groups and as a species, we do not learn at the rate of one human being’s year per year. So when we hear from academia that our species is 150,000 years old, and our line is around 4 million years old — we should realize that these figures actually imply something quite different. To understand how old we are, we’d need to do something like take each of those 150k years, and multiply it by individual human lives to determine a humans-per-year velocity of time-relation. But if we did this, we’d be making a mistake — were humans separate from other life forms? From the Earth itself, as an organism?

We will explore such questions in process. For the moment, we may observe that as a species, we are older than we think. In 2003, one year on Earth results in over 6 billion life-years of human experience. That is a significant acceleration of complexity accrual, especially when we observe that as cognitive animals humans are evolutionarily elaborating a new domain in planetary expression — one that is symbolically representational, and systemically conserved. We should remind ourselves often that small catastrophes can crush such conservations, and probably have more than once in our history as a species.

So we are older than anything we commonly imagine, as a species. Yet what we are comprised from, the domains of scale that coallesce in us are infinitely older even than us for two reasons — time passes faster for them (vastly) and their populations make ours seem like a speck. We divide Time into ourselves, to gain evolutionary speed where it is physically impossible to do so. But cells and their associations are doing this on a scale that renders human evolution into a crawl, even with the adaptations of language and technology.

We use population, to change what Time means, in evolutionary terms. We are timeCreatures. And thus it is with all the scales of life. Any extant species is older than the local organization of the Sun — even though the Sun ‘was here first’. How much each of the members of a species shares in this resource is open to question, but we cannot believe this is not what is happening. Each of us, each living thing, represents the forefront of a wave reaching back (at least) to the first moments of Life on Earth.

At the same time, we are not even children yet. Language is changing us faster than any organism in Earth’s history changed, and while it is changing us, it is changing Earth. Because of the rapidity of this change, there is no state of ‘adulthood’ in such an equation, the target is moving too quickly, and everything is being changed by this movement. But in terms of human generations, what we are today is most likely far far younger than we imagine.

The consciousness we experience today in the West is primarily the product of a scant 10 human generations of 50 year lifespans. Those 10 sit atop another 50 that take us back 3000 years the one of the likely moments of emergence for the progenitors of the simulatory consciousness which is our experience today. Seen in this light our lack of commonly accessible memory for events only 50 generations ago is fascinating. It seems the transition from living reality to myth takes only some 20 or 30 generations — a surprisingly tiny span.

When I was a young man, listening to the stories of my parents, grandparents, and occasionally great-grandparents it occurred to me that as one went back even a scant few generations, what people thought and how they acted and lived appeared to become more unsane with each generation of travel backward. Perhaps this is a reflection of my own lineage or speculative biases, but there is evidence that my youthful intuitions were more correct than I might have imagined. 4 generations ago, the world was indeed a very different place, and what it was to be alive within it was also radically different.

A mere 60 generations ago, human beings had minds very different from our own. Their experience of what it was to be human was unlike ours in significant ways, and their experiences of language and expression also differed dramatically. Perhaps as little as 500 years ago, language was primarily something one made or heard. In many places, it still is. Writing and reading are actually highly specialized skills, and our relationship with them as a species is far newer than we imagine, for the phases of this relationship’s arisal are not alike with our systematic and statistical understandings — they are instead the record of something more like a penetration, by something that was then and still remains as alien and threatening as it is empowering.

We are sitting upon a mountain of unexamined history which represents of the ladder of our rise to representational sentience. We do not know what happened in the garden where our sentience and our ability to metaphy was born. Nor do we know why we do not know. The answer given by most is that it happened too long ago — somewhere between 150,000 and 40,000 years ago. This seems enough to quiet the questions in most of us. There are exceptions, however. It’s actually possible that most our complex cognitive accruals, at least in the domain of consciousness, are actually of a much more recent origin than we suspect.

Our models, stories and general positions of relation to our metaphors imply we are sophisticated, and we are — but perhaps not in the way we imagine. When we invert the bubble of understanding and glimpse the real terrain there we will inevitably discover an entirely new universe. Most of what is here to be explored, is ignored. It’s hiding behind labels, and systems of knowing.

 

o:O:o

 

:: previous :: next ::

 

 

“The way that language represents objects, events, and relationships provides a uniquely powerful economy of reference. It offers a means for generating an essentially infinite variety of novel representations, and an unprecedented inferential engine for predicting events, organizing memories, and planning behaviors. It entirely shapes our thinking and the ways we know the physical world. It is so pervasive and inseparable from human intelligence in general that it is difficult to distinguish what aspects of the human intellect have not been molded and streamlined by it. To explain this difference and describe the evolutionary circumstances that brought it about are the ultimate challenges in the study of human origins.”

— Terrence W. Deacon, The Symbolic Species:
The Co-Evolution of Language and the Brain